Publication Date
In 2025 | 16 |
Since 2024 | 81 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 303 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 693 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 1293 |
Descriptor
Source
Author
Treiman, Rebecca | 31 |
Downing, John | 23 |
Justice, Laura M. | 20 |
Aram, Dorit | 14 |
Perea, Manuel | 12 |
Hulme, Charles | 11 |
Sanders, Elizabeth A. | 11 |
Al Otaiba, Stephanie | 10 |
Vadasy, Patricia F. | 10 |
Ehri, Linnea C. | 9 |
Kim, Young-Suk | 9 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 429 |
Elementary Education | 423 |
Primary Education | 260 |
Kindergarten | 245 |
Preschool Education | 162 |
Grade 1 | 159 |
Higher Education | 111 |
Postsecondary Education | 81 |
Grade 2 | 78 |
Grade 3 | 51 |
Grade 4 | 50 |
More ▼ |
Audience
Teachers | 88 |
Practitioners | 77 |
Students | 58 |
Researchers | 20 |
Parents | 10 |
Administrators | 5 |
Media Staff | 4 |
Community | 1 |
Location
Australia | 35 |
Canada | 35 |
Israel | 29 |
California | 28 |
Turkey | 25 |
China | 24 |
United Kingdom (England) | 23 |
Netherlands | 21 |
India | 20 |
Florida | 18 |
France | 18 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Meets WWC Standards without Reservations | 7 |
Meets WWC Standards with or without Reservations | 7 |
Does not meet standards | 5 |

Mason, Mildred – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1982
Three experiments report additional evidence that it is a mistake to account for all interletter effects solely in terms of sensory variables. These experiments attest to the importance of structural variables such as retina location, array size, and ordinal position. (Author/PN)
Descriptors: Associative Learning, Cognitive Processes, Eye Fixations, Higher Education

Visible Language, 1979
Describes research projects and experiments in graphic design conducted at a number of colleges and universities. (GT)
Descriptors: Alphabets, Design, Educational Games, Experiments

Dermott, R. Allan; And Others – Reading Improvement, 1980
Concludes that, of several reading readiness factors, knowledge of alphabet letter names is the best predictor of phonics skills other than vowel recognition and of reading abilities involving words in insolation, and that number knowledge is the best predictor of reading abilities involving words and syntax. (FL)
Descriptors: Grade 1, Letters (Alphabet), Number Concepts, Phonics

Chase, Christopher H.; Tallal, Paula – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1990
Examined effects of orthographic context on the letter recognition skills of dyslexic children, comparing their performance to that of adults and of chronological and reading age-matched groups. Results showed that the two matched groups showed strong word superiority effect (WSE) for words and pseudowords over nonwords. Dyslexic readers did not…
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Children, Cognitive Processes

Rohl, Mary; Tunmer, William E. – Applied Psycholinguistics, 1988
Reponses of poor, average, and good spellers at different age levels to a phonemic segmentation test containing nondigraph pseudowords and to an experimental spelling test containing exception, ambiguous, regular, and pseudowords suggested that the average and good spellers made fewer and more phonetically accurate errors than the poor spellers.…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Elementary Education, Error Analysis (Language), Grade 2

Lukatela, Katerina; And Others – Applied Psycholinguistics, 1995
Assesses illiterate and semiliterate speakers of Serbo-Croatian on reading, writing, phonological, and control tasks. Three groups, categorized with respect to the subjects' ability to identify the letters of their Cyrillic alphabet, differed on phoneme deletion and phoneme-counting tasks, but not on syllable-counting, picture vocabulary, or…
Descriptors: Cyrillic Alphabet, Foreign Countries, Functional Literacy, Illiteracy

Aissing, Alena L. – Information Technology and Libraries, 1992
Discusses bibliographic access to documents in nonroman scripts and the need for standards for the way different scripts are encoded for data processing. A microcomputer-based system that transliterates Cyrillic text to a romanized version and back to Cyrillic is proposed. (10 references) (MES)
Descriptors: Access to Information, Cataloging, Computer Software, Cyrillic Alphabet

Muter, Valerie; Hulme, Charles; Snowling, Margaret; Taylor, Sara – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1998
Examined phonological skills of children during their first two years of learning to read. Found that segmentation was strongly correlated with reading and spelling attainment at the end of the first year of school; letter-name knowledge predicted reading and spelling skill and interacted with segmentation skills. Rhyming predicted spelling skills…
Descriptors: Emergent Literacy, Knowledge Level, Letters (Alphabet), Longitudinal Studies

Liow, Susan Rikard; Green, David; Tam, Melissa – International Journal of Bilingualism, 1999
To test whether cognitive processing in bilingual depends on script combinations and language proficiency, this study investigated the development of alphabetic and logographic visual search strategies in two kinds of biscriptals: (1) Malay-English and (2) Chinese-English readers. Results support the view that there are script implications of…
Descriptors: Bilingualism, Chinese, Cognitive Processes, Contrastive Linguistics
Caravolas, Marketa; Volin, Jan; Hulme, Charles – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2005
Two studies investigated the importance of phoneme awareness relative to other predictors in the development of reading and spelling among children learning a consistent orthography (Czech) and an inconsistent orthography (English). In Study 1, structural equation models revealed that Czech (n=107) and English (n=71) data were fitted well by the…
Descriptors: Phonemes, Structural Equation Models, Slavic Languages, Spelling
Berninger, Virginia W.; Rutberg, Judith E.; Abbott, Robert D.; Garcia, Noelia; Anderson-Youngstrom, Marci; Brooks, Allison; Fulton, Cynthia – Journal of School Psychology, 2006
Three studies evaluated Tier 1 early intervention for handwriting at a critical period for literacy development in first grade and one study evaluated Tier 2 early intervention in the critical period between third and fourth grades for composing on high stakes tests. The results contribute to knowledge of research-supported handwriting and…
Descriptors: Early Intervention, Handwriting, Writing (Composition), Writing Instruction
Moore, Daniele – International Journal of Multilingualism, 2006
This contribution documents the strategies bi/plurilingual children spontaneously develop when confronted with a task that demands comprehension of texts in a third language. To illustrate these strategies in context, the paper draws from classroom interaction excerpts: small groups of children (ages 8-10) discover meaning in a text in a language…
Descriptors: Metalinguistics, Multilingualism, Mandarin Chinese, Learning Strategies
What Works Clearinghouse, 2007
"Doors to Discovery"[TM] is an early childhood curriculum that uses thematic units to engage young children and support them as they build an understanding of their world. "Doors to Discovery"[TM] literacy activities are used to encourage children's development in a number of areas identified by research as the foundation for…
Descriptors: Program Effectiveness, Disadvantaged Youth, Early Reading, Intervention
Hendricks, Charlene; Trueblood, Linda; Pasnak, Robert – Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 2006
Seven-year-olds who had difficulty understanding 1st-grade work received one of two forms of small-group instruction. Half of the children were randomly assigned to receive four months of instruction in recognizing, comprehending, and reproducing both logical and arbitrary patterns (sequences) involving numbers, letters, shapes, colors,…
Descriptors: Small Group Instruction, Academic Achievement, Grade 1, Elementary School Students
Pemberton, Elizabeth; And Others – 1993
The validity of the suggestion that dyslexic children make more letter reversal errors than other children was tested. Horizontal letter reversals of 8- to 11-year-old dyslexic children, nondyslexic speech- or language-impaired (SLI) children, and nonimpaired children in the context of individual letters, words, words spelled out letter-by-letter,…
Descriptors: Children, Comparative Analysis, Dyslexia, Elementary Education