Publication Date
In 2025 | 13 |
Since 2024 | 78 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 212 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 471 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 882 |
Descriptor
Source
Author
Publication Type
Education Level
Audience
Practitioners | 188 |
Teachers | 124 |
Students | 44 |
Researchers | 38 |
Parents | 2 |
Administrators | 1 |
Community | 1 |
Media Staff | 1 |
Location
China | 33 |
Australia | 28 |
Canada | 24 |
Indonesia | 24 |
Japan | 24 |
Netherlands | 18 |
South Korea | 18 |
Germany | 15 |
Taiwan | 15 |
Turkey | 15 |
United Kingdom (England) | 15 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
National Defense Education… | 4 |
Elementary and Secondary… | 2 |
Bilingual Education Act 1968 | 1 |
Elementary and Secondary… | 1 |
Head Start | 1 |
No Child Left Behind Act 2001 | 1 |
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Meets WWC Standards without Reservations | 2 |
Meets WWC Standards with or without Reservations | 2 |

Cornulier, Benoit de – Langue Francaise, 1979
Analyzes the distribution of information in so-called "synonymous" sentences in the context of a semantic perspective. (AM)
Descriptors: Discourse Analysis, Grammar, Pragmatics, Semantics
Cook, Walter A. – Georgetown Journal of Languages and Linguistics, 1990
Presents clues for determining whether "short passives," passive sentences from which the "by"-phrase has been deleted, are actually passive or stative. The clues include intensifiers, the verb "seems," contrasts, present tense, past tense, progressive aspect, and confirmation by examples. (CB)
Descriptors: English, Prepositions, Semantics, Sentence Structure
Cook, Walter A. – Georgetown Journal of Languages and Linguistics, 1990
Examines the idiomatic meaning carried in a sentence through the syntactic idiom. The infinitive of negative result is used to examine the understanding of the meaning carried in a sentence by its syntactic structure. (GLR)
Descriptors: Idioms, Negative Forms (Language), Sentence Structure, Surface Structure

Espinal, M. Teresa – Language, 1991
Describes the interaction between disjunct constituents and sentential structures and argues for an extension of the theoretical apparatus intially postulated in autosegmental phonology to represent complex syntactic structures containing disjunct constituents with a number of independent phrase markers that intersect at the linear axis. (66…
Descriptors: Linguistic Theory, Phonology, Sentence Structure, Structural Analysis (Linguistics)

May, John D. – Language and Communication, 1991
It is proposed that declarative sentences used in ordinary speech deliver regular, patterned suggestions. The reader or auditor is invited to infer that the utterance is coherent, pertinent, feasible, true, and believed by the speaker and that the speaker could have ascertained its truth. (41 references) (MSE)
Descriptors: Grammar, Interpersonal Communication, Semantics, Sentence Structure

Pickering, Martin; Barry, Guy – Language and Cognitive Processes, 1991
Provides evidence that sentence processing does not make use of grammatical theories with empty categories. A linguistic account is provided of unbounded dependencies that do not use empty categories and can serve as the basis of a processing model. It is concluded that empty categories are not psychologically real. (28 references) (JL)
Descriptors: Grammar, Language Processing, Linguistic Theory, Models

Lerner, Gene H. – Language in Society, 1991
Describes how two conversants can jointly produce a single syntactic unit, such as a sentence, discussing the types of sentences achieved through joint production, the single utterance construction format, syntax for conversation, interactionally relevant features of talk, and the resources needed to complete an utterance-in-progress. (37…
Descriptors: English, Language Processing, Oral Language, Sentence Structure

Hedberg, Nancy – Language, 2000
Shows how the subject pronoun in a cleft sentence together with the cleft clause function pragmatically as a discontinuous definite description. Presents a new syntactic analysis of clefts, which treats the cleft clause as an extraposed complement of the cleft subject pronoun, adjoined to the clefted constituent. (Author/VWL)
Descriptors: Pragmatics, Pronouns, Sentence Structure, Structural Analysis (Linguistics)
Montelongo, Jose A.; Hernandez, Anita C. – Reading Teacher, 2007
This article introduces a modification of the sentence completion task that acquaints students with various types of informational text structures (e.g., cause-effect, compare-contrast) and challenges their higher order reading and writing skills. When the individual sentences comprising an expository paragraph are intermingled with unrelated…
Descriptors: Grade 5, Grade 4, Writing Skills, Sentences
Altmann, Lori J. P.; Kemper, Susan – Language and Cognitive Processes, 2006
The current study examines whether young and older adults have similar preferences for animate-subject and active sentences, and for using the order of activation of a verb's arguments to determine sentence structure. Ninety-six participants produced sentences in response to three-word stimuli that included a verb and two nouns differing in…
Descriptors: Verbs, Older Adults, Young Adults, Nouns
Christianson, Kiel; Williams, Carrick C.; Zacks, Rose T.; Ferreira, Fernanda – Discourse Processes: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 2006
We report 3 experiments that examined younger and older adults' reliance on "good-enough" interpretations for garden-path sentences (e.g., "While Anna dressed the baby played in the crib") as indicated by their responding "Yes" to questions probing the initial, syntactically unlicensed interpretation (e.g., "Did Anna dress the baby?"). The…
Descriptors: Verbs, Sentence Structure, Probability, Age Differences
Hale, John – Cognitive Science, 2006
A word-by-word human sentence processing complexity metric is presented. This metric formalizes the intuition that comprehenders have more trouble on words contributing larger amounts of information about the syntactic structure of the sentence as a whole. The formalization is in terms of the conditional entropy of grammatical continuations, given…
Descriptors: Sentences, Sentence Structure, Grammar, Prediction
Kaschak, Michael P.; Loney, Renrick A.; Borreggine, Kristin L. – Cognition, 2006
In two experiments, we explore how recent experience with particular syntactic constructions affects the strength of the structural priming observed for those constructions. The results suggest that (1) the strength of structural priming observed for double object and prepositional object constructions is affected by the relative frequency with…
Descriptors: Experiments, Effect Size, Experiments, Language Processing
Gibson, Edward – Journal of Memory and Language, 2006
This paper investigates how people resolve syntactic category ambiguities when comprehending sentences. It is proposed that people combine: (a) context-dependent syntactic expectations (top-down statistical information) and (b) context-independent lexical-category frequencies of words (bottom-up statistical information) in order to resolve…
Descriptors: Figurative Language, Sentence Structure, Language Acquisition, Models
MacKay, Donald G.; James, Lori E.; Taylor, Jennifer K.; Marian, Diane E. – Language and Cognitive Processes, 2007
This study examines sentence-level language abilities of amnesic H.M. to test competing theoretical conceptions of relations between language and memory. We present 11 new sources of experimental evidence indicating deficits in H.M's comprehension and production of non-cliche sentences. Contrary to recent claims that H.M.'s comprehension is…
Descriptors: Language Processing, Sentences, Sentence Structure, Grammar