Publication Date
In 2025 | 17 |
Since 2024 | 82 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 304 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 695 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 1295 |
Descriptor
Source
Author
Treiman, Rebecca | 31 |
Downing, John | 23 |
Justice, Laura M. | 20 |
Aram, Dorit | 14 |
Perea, Manuel | 12 |
Hulme, Charles | 11 |
Sanders, Elizabeth A. | 11 |
Al Otaiba, Stephanie | 10 |
Vadasy, Patricia F. | 10 |
Ehri, Linnea C. | 9 |
Kim, Young-Suk | 9 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 429 |
Elementary Education | 423 |
Primary Education | 260 |
Kindergarten | 245 |
Preschool Education | 162 |
Grade 1 | 159 |
Higher Education | 112 |
Postsecondary Education | 82 |
Grade 2 | 78 |
Grade 3 | 51 |
Grade 4 | 50 |
More ▼ |
Audience
Teachers | 88 |
Practitioners | 77 |
Students | 58 |
Researchers | 20 |
Parents | 10 |
Administrators | 5 |
Media Staff | 4 |
Community | 1 |
Location
Australia | 35 |
Canada | 35 |
Israel | 29 |
California | 28 |
Turkey | 25 |
China | 24 |
United Kingdom (England) | 23 |
Netherlands | 21 |
India | 20 |
Florida | 18 |
France | 18 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Meets WWC Standards without Reservations | 7 |
Meets WWC Standards with or without Reservations | 7 |
Does not meet standards | 5 |

Naus, Mary J.; Shillman, Robert J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1976
Although a feature detection theory of pattern recognition is consistent with many recent physiological findings, the specific rules governing the perception of the distinctive features of letters have not yet been determined. This article presents two new experimental procedures for determining these rules. (Editor)
Descriptors: Charts, Experimental Psychology, Letters (Alphabet), Orthographic Symbols
Hecaen, H. – Langages, 1976
This article discusses the role of neurological damage in the form of lesions, as it relates to alexia. (Text is in French.) (CLK)
Descriptors: Anatomy, Aphasia, Eyes, Language Research

Roberts, Theresa A. – Journal of Educational Psychology, 2003
Examines the influence of letter-name instruction in beginning word recognition. Children who received letter-name instruction learned words phonetically spelled with letters included in instruction significantly better than other words. Children receiving comprehension instruction performed significantly better on visually distinct word…
Descriptors: Instructional Effectiveness, Letters (Alphabet), Phonetics, Preschool Children

Speece, Deborah L.; Mills, Christina; Ritchey, Kristen D.; Hillman, Elgen – Journal of Special Education, 2003
This study examined the validity of letter-name fluency and nonsense word fluency (NWF) measures as indicators of early reading skill with 39 kindergarten children who were tested again in first grade. Evidence supportive of validity was found for both measures, with NWF receiving the strongest support and both fluency measures more sensitive than…
Descriptors: Early Identification, Kindergarten, Letters (Alphabet), Predictive Validity

Gonzalez-Bueno, Manuela – Reading Teacher, 2003
Presents some examples of bilingual and Spanish materials that can be prepared in the classroom based on current practical theories on the teaching of literacy. Describes ways to incorporate the alphabet into bilingual activities. Considers the use of an illustrated songbook. (SG)
Descriptors: Alphabets, Bilingual Students, Class Activities, Elementary Education

Veatch, Jeannette – Journal of Reading Education, 1988
Argues that direct instruction can be democratic when the content comes from the pupil. Argues that minimal reading achievement levels result from using text-centered, behavioral-objective-based, criterion-referenced, and profitable commercial reading programs. Asserts that students' writing improves when they internalize the alphabet. (RS)
Descriptors: Elementary Education, Letters (Alphabet), Reading Instruction, Reading Processes

Bjaalid, Inger-Kristine; And Others – Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 1993
Accuracy of letter identification and latency of reaction time were studied as a function of retinal position in 19 dyslexic adolescents. Dyslexics did not show higher performance scores in the peripheral visual fields but did have longer reaction times in all experimental conditions. (SLD)
Descriptors: Adolescents, Dyslexia, Eyes, Identification

Graham, Steve – Focus on Exceptional Children, 1992
This paper examines the importance of handwriting instruction, through discussion of four issues: (1) whether handwriting should be taught directly outside other writing contexts; (2) slanted versus traditional manuscript alphabets; (3) use of a beginner's pencil and wide lined paper; and (4) the use of self-regulation procedures within the…
Descriptors: Alphabets, Elementary Education, Handwriting, Self Evaluation (Individuals)
Vidor, Constance – School Library Media Activities Monthly, 1994
Proposes four subcategories of alphabet books for use in upper elementary classrooms: enumerative, juxtapositional, narrative, and artistic. Characteristics of each type are described that encourage different kinds of reader involvement and examples of classroom uses and strategies are suggested for specific titles. (LRW)
Descriptors: Alphabets, Childrens Literature, Class Activities, Elementary Education

Berninger, Virginia W.; Vaughan, Katherine; Abbott, Robert D.; Brooks, Allison; Begay, Kristin; Curtin, Gerald; Byrd, Kristina; Graham, Steve – Learning Disability Quarterly, 2000
Two studies compared the effectiveness of alphabet principle training only versus combined alphabet principle and syllable awareness training with at-risk spellers (grades 2 and 3). Differing results suggest use of a two-tier early intervention model in which first the alphabet principle is taught and applied and then the following year children…
Descriptors: Alphabets, Early Intervention, Learning Problems, Primary Education

Riley, Jeni L. – Journal of Research in Reading, 1996
Finds that children's ability to identify and label the letters of the alphabet and to write their own name at school entry were the most powerful predictors of successful reading by the end of the year. Finds a weaker, but still positive, relationship between understanding the conventions of print and reading achievement. (RS)
Descriptors: Early Childhood Education, Letters (Alphabet), Predictor Variables, Reading Achievement

Neuhaus, Graham F.; Swank, Paul R. – Journal of Learning Disabilities, 2002
First grade students (n=221) were tested on measures of verbal fluency, visual attention, phonological awareness, orthographic recognition, rapid automated naming (RAN) of letters and objects, and reading. Findings indicated that word reading was directly and significantly predicted by RAN letter naming and general RAN cognitive processing time of…
Descriptors: Alphabets, Beginning Reading, Cognitive Processes, Grade 1

Graham, Steve; Weintraub, Naomi; Berninger, Virginia – Journal of Educational Psychology, 2001
Examined manuscript letter writing skills of students in Grades 1-3. Three letter characteristics, grade, and alphabet fluency each made a significant contribution to the prediction of letter legibility after effects of other predictors were controlled. Letter legibility in turn made a significant contribution to the prediction of text legibility…
Descriptors: Beginning Writing, Childrens Writing, Handwriting, Letters (Alphabet)

Green, Connie R. – Childhood Education, 1998
Notes that names are the first words most children write and that learning to write their name can be highly motivating for preschoolers. Addresses: why preschool children should be encouraged to write their names; organizing and facilitating the sign-in process at school; how children develop their ability to write their names; and the benefits…
Descriptors: Handwriting, Language Acquisition, Letters (Alphabet), Preschool Education
de Jong, P. F.; Olson, R. K. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2004
This study examined the influence of phonological memory and rapid naming on the development of letter knowledge. Participants were 77 Dutch children, who were followed from the start of their first kindergarten year (mean age 4 years 6.8 months) to the end of their second kindergarten year. Phonological memory was assessed by a nonword repetition…
Descriptors: Phonology, Short Term Memory, Influences, Predictor Variables