Publication Date
In 2025 | 198 |
Since 2024 | 861 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 2246 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 3867 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 7442 |
Descriptor
Source
Author
Publication Type
Education Level
Audience
Practitioners | 182 |
Teachers | 144 |
Researchers | 129 |
Administrators | 14 |
Parents | 12 |
Students | 9 |
Counselors | 4 |
Policymakers | 4 |
Community | 1 |
Support Staff | 1 |
Location
China | 195 |
Germany | 128 |
Australia | 116 |
Canada | 112 |
United Kingdom | 108 |
Netherlands | 99 |
Japan | 98 |
Spain | 94 |
France | 62 |
Hong Kong | 62 |
Iran | 60 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
No Child Left Behind Act 2001 | 5 |
Education Consolidation… | 1 |
Head Start | 1 |
Individuals with Disabilities… | 1 |
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Meets WWC Standards without Reservations | 1 |
Meets WWC Standards with or without Reservations | 2 |
Does not meet standards | 3 |
Hernán Rosario Rivera – ProQuest LLC, 2023
Code-switching has been studied from different approaches, ranging from syntactical, sociolinguistic, psycholinguistic, phonetically and many others. One key area that phonetic studies have explore has been the production of VOT in Spanish-English bilinguals due to the differences across the languages. To understand how bilinguals co-activate…
Descriptors: Code Switching (Language), Native Language, Second Language Learning, Second Language Instruction
Reima Al-Jarf – Online Submission, 2023
Time metaphorical expressions are common in all languages and in general as well as specialized contexts. This study explores the similarities and differences between English and Arabic time metaphorical expressions containing , and the difficulties that student-translators have in translating them; the translation strategies they use and the…
Descriptors: Time, Figurative Language, Arabic, English (Second Language)
Nurit Gur-Yaish; Sujoud Hijazy; Eden Mazareeb; Mila Schwartz – Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 2023
Limited research to date has addressed the language socialisation of transnational children during global movement with their parents. The aim of this study was to explore the complexity of language socialisation for a transnational child experiencing a multilingual environment both at home and in preschool. This transnational English- and…
Descriptors: Hebrew, English, Arabic, Multilingualism
James, Lori E.; Schmank, Christopher J.; Castro, Nichol; Buchanan, Tony W. – Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2018
We tested the frequent assumption that the difficulty of word retrieval increases when a speaker is being observed and evaluated. We modified the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) so that participants believed that its evaluative observation components continued throughout the duration of a subsequent word retrieval task, and measured participants'…
Descriptors: Models, Psycholinguistics, Language Processing, Stress Variables
Shen, Wei; Li, Zhao; Tong, Xiuhong – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 2018
Purpose: This study aimed to investigate the time course of meaning activation of the 2nd morpheme processing of compound words during Chinese spoken word recognition using eye tracking technique with the printed-word paradigm. Method: In the printed-word paradigm, participants were instructed to listen to a spoken target word (e.g., [Chinese…
Descriptors: Morphemes, Language Processing, Syllables, Word Recognition
Domaneschi, Filippo; Di Paola, Simona – Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2018
The present study investigates the processing of presupposition accommodation. In particular, it concerns the processing costs and the time-course of accommodation as compared to presupposition satisfaction. Data collected in a self-paced word-by-word reading times experiment support three results. First, independently on the presupposition…
Descriptors: Language Processing, Comparative Analysis, Reading Rate, Sentences
Liu, Pingping; Lu, Qin – Journal of Research in Reading, 2018
This paper studies the mechanisms behind the differential effects of inserting a space either before or after a two-character unit on information processing through the examination of eye movements in Chinese, a language where there is no word delimiters. A two-character unit in this study is either a word-preserving stimulus or a word-disrupting…
Descriptors: Eye Movements, Chinese, Reading Processes, Orthographic Symbols
Clahsen, Harald; Felser, Claudia – Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 2018
Since the Shallow Structure Hypothesis (SSH) was first put forward in 2006, it has inspired a growing body of research on grammatical processing in nonnative (L2) speakers. More than 10 years later, we think it is time for the SSH to be reconsidered in the light of new empirical findings and current theoretical assumptions about human language…
Descriptors: Linguistic Theory, Second Language Learning, Language Processing, Native Language
Gregg, Julie; Sajin, Stanislav – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2018
Across two visual world paradigm (VWP) experiments, Salverda and Tanenhaus (2010) observed an effect of orthographic overlap between targets and competitors in the absence of an effect of phonological overlap when mapping spoken targets onto briefly previewed printed arrays. They concluded that the use of orthographic knowledge can precede use of…
Descriptors: Phonology, Eye Movements, Speech, Undergraduate Students
Sanchez-Ferreres, Josep; Delicado, Luis; Andaloussi, Amine Abbab; Burattin, Andrea; Calderon-Ruiz, Guillermo; Weber, Barbara; Carmona, Josep; Padro, Lluis – IEEE Transactions on Learning Technologies, 2020
The creation of a process model is primarily a formalization task that faces the challenge of constructing a syntactically correct entity, which accurately reflects the semantics of reality, and is understandable to the model reader. This article proposes a framework called "Model Judge," focused toward the two main actors in the process…
Descriptors: Models, Automation, Validity, Natural Language Processing
Shetreet, Einat; Novogrodsky, Rama – Language Learning and Development, 2020
Universal quantifiers, which refer to groups of individuals or events, can express a subtle distinction between collective (unified or simultaneous) and distributive (individuated and separate) events. Indeed, English uses different quantifiers for this distinction ("all" and "each", respectively). Hebrew, however, has a single…
Descriptors: Morphology (Languages), Form Classes (Languages), Semitic Languages, Syntax
Foucart, Alice; Costa, Albert; Morís-Fernández, Luis; Hartsuiker, Robert J. – Language Learning, 2020
The extent to which negative bias toward foreign-accented speakers originates from social categorization (in-group/out-group categorization) and/or from processing fluency (ease in processing information) is not clear. Some have argued that accent first induces a social identity effect and that processing fluency later modifies the impact of this…
Descriptors: Pronunciation, Social Bias, Language Attitudes, Language Processing
Desmeules-Trudel, Félix; Moore, Charlotte; Zamuner, Tania S. – Journal of Child Language, 2020
Bilingual children cope with a significant amount of phonetic variability when processing speech, and must learn to weigh phonetic cues differently depending on the cues' respective roles in their two languages. For example, vowel nasalization is coarticulatory and contrastive in French, but coarticulatory-only in English. In this study, we…
Descriptors: Monolingualism, Bilingualism, Children, Young Children
Hartshorne, Joshua K. – First Language, 2020
Ambridge argues that the existence of exemplar models for individual phenomena (words, inflection rules, etc.) suggests the feasibility of a unified, exemplars-everywhere model that eschews abstraction. The argument would be strengthened by a description of such a model. However, none is provided. I show that any attempt to do so would immediately…
Descriptors: Models, Language Acquisition, Language Processing, Bayesian Statistics
Hupp, Julie M.; Jungers, Melissa K.; Porter, Brandon L.; Plunkett, Brandy A. – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2020
When hearing an object label, a specific object may come to mind. With the phrase, "There was a balloon in the pack/air" the representation of balloon varies based on the implied shape (deflated vs. inflated). The current study investigated whether the implied shape affects sentence-picture verification for adults and preschool children.…
Descriptors: Adults, Preschool Children, Age Differences, Sentences