Publication Date
In 2025 | 16 |
Since 2024 | 81 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 303 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 693 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 1293 |
Descriptor
Source
Author
Treiman, Rebecca | 31 |
Downing, John | 23 |
Justice, Laura M. | 20 |
Aram, Dorit | 14 |
Perea, Manuel | 12 |
Hulme, Charles | 11 |
Sanders, Elizabeth A. | 11 |
Al Otaiba, Stephanie | 10 |
Vadasy, Patricia F. | 10 |
Ehri, Linnea C. | 9 |
Kim, Young-Suk | 9 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 429 |
Elementary Education | 423 |
Primary Education | 260 |
Kindergarten | 245 |
Preschool Education | 162 |
Grade 1 | 159 |
Higher Education | 111 |
Postsecondary Education | 81 |
Grade 2 | 78 |
Grade 3 | 51 |
Grade 4 | 50 |
More ▼ |
Audience
Teachers | 88 |
Practitioners | 77 |
Students | 58 |
Researchers | 20 |
Parents | 10 |
Administrators | 5 |
Media Staff | 4 |
Community | 1 |
Location
Australia | 35 |
Canada | 35 |
Israel | 29 |
California | 28 |
Turkey | 25 |
China | 24 |
United Kingdom (England) | 23 |
Netherlands | 21 |
India | 20 |
Florida | 18 |
France | 18 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Meets WWC Standards without Reservations | 7 |
Meets WWC Standards with or without Reservations | 7 |
Does not meet standards | 5 |

Flowers, J. H.; And Others – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1981
Familiar letter sequences in noncued portions of a tachistoscopic display were shown to reduce accuracy of partial report. Findings suggest that familiarity may automatically direct attentional resources to a particular spatial region. Such attentional capture may be disruptive if the material is presented at another location. (Author/RD)
Descriptors: Attention Control, Cognitive Processes, Cues, Higher Education

Rumelhart, David E.; McClelland, James L. – Psychological Review, 1982
The duration and timing of the context is which letters occur is shown to influence the perceptibility of the target in experiments demonstrating that early on enhanced word presentations and pronounceable-pseudoword contexts increase letter perceptibility. The perceptibility of letters in strings sharing several or few letters with words is…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Context Clues, Context Effect, Higher Education

Adams, Marilyn Jager – Cognitive Psychology, 1979
Hypotheses about the processes involved in word recognition are reviewed and assessed through four experiments. Overall results were compatible with criterion bias models. A version of this model attributes the advantage of words (over pseudowords and nonwords) to interfacilitation among single letter and lexical units in memory. (Author/RD)
Descriptors: Adults, Cognitive Processes, Letters (Alphabet), Orthographic Symbols

Guttentag, Robert E.; Haith, Marshall M. – Journal of Educational Psychology, 1980
Twelve first-grade children were tested on word-reading and automaticity of letter and word processing. Word-reading speed increased steadily during the year. Apparently, their ability to process letters automatically was acquired prior to the ability to read words rapidly and accurately. (Author/CP)
Descriptors: Beginning Reading, Grade 1, Letters (Alphabet), Pictorial Stimuli

Ehri, Linnea C.; Wilce, Lee S. – Journal of Educational Psychology, 1979
Mnemonic value of spellings in a paired-associate sound learning task was examined in first and second graders. Learning was fastest when correct spellings were seen or imagined. The preferred interpretation was that spellings are effective because they provide readers with orthographic images for symbolizing and storing sounds in memory.…
Descriptors: Early Reading, Learning Processes, Letters (Alphabet), Mnemonics

Oldrieve, Richard M. – TEACHING Exceptional Children, 1997
Describes the structured internalization spelling method, which uses a series of small, graduated steps to teach students with learning disabilities to transcribe phonological sounds (phonemes) as alphabetic letters (graphemes) onto paper. The implementation of the program and the benefits of structured internalization are presented along with a…
Descriptors: Elementary Secondary Education, Graphemes, Learning Disabilities, Lesson Plans

Gamage, Gayathri Hathhotuwa – Australian Review of Applied Linguistics, 2003
Investigates three important issues in kanji learning strategies; namely, strategy use, effectiveness of strategy and orthographic background. A questionnaire on kanji learning strategy use and perceived effectiveness was administered to beginner level, undergraduate students of Japanese from alphabetic and character backgrounds in Australia.…
Descriptors: Chinese, College Students, Higher Education, Japanese

Tuman, Walter Vladimir – Foreign Language Annals, 1990
Discusses the "Russian Dynamic Hand," a working prototype for introducing the Russian writing and sound system that has been available to students at Louisiana State University since September 1988. The project's evolution is discussed from an initial graphics-centered IBM prototype to a Macintosh version enhanced by an audio component.…
Descriptors: Computer Assisted Instruction, Cyrillic Alphabet, Higher Education, Introductory Courses

Treiman, Rebecca; And Others – Applied Psycholinguistics, 1994
Three studies examined preschoolers' and kindergartners' learning of correspondences between phonemes and graphemes. Findings suggest that children use their knowledge of letter names and their phonological segmentation skills rather than memorizing these links in a rote, paired-associate manner. (Contains 26 references.) (Author/LB)
Descriptors: Child Language, Foreign Countries, Graphemes, Language Acquisition

Bowey, Judith A. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1994
Study examined whether phonemic sensitivity is limited to alphabetically literate individuals. Children not exposed to reading instruction were given pairs of phonological sensitivity tasks. Novice readers scored higher in phonological sensitivity than nonreaders of equivalent letter knowledge, when controlled for verbal ability; among nonreaders,…
Descriptors: Early Reading, Foreign Countries, Letters (Alphabet), Phonemic Awareness
Stull, Elizabeth C. – Book Links, 1998
Presents a descriptive bibliography of children's alphabet books under the categories of: Animals and Nature, Cultures and Holidays, and Notable Graphics. Suggests activities for primary and intermediate grades which use these materials. (AEF)
Descriptors: Annotated Bibliographies, Childrens Literature, Educational Resources, Elementary Education

McBride-Chang, Catherine – Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 1999
This study investigated the development of alphabet knowledge among 91 nonreading children followed from the beginning of kindergarten through the middle of first grade. Findings suggested that alphabet knowledge consists of both letter-name and letter-sound knowledge, which have different developmental patterns and which have unique importance…
Descriptors: Beginning Reading, Grade 1, Graphemes, Kindergarten
Ho'omanawanui, Ku'ualoha – American Indian Quarterly, 2004
Until American Calvinist missionaries created a Hawaiian alphabet and writing system after their arrival to the Hawaiian islands in 1819, all Kanaka Maoli literature was oral. By inventing a Hawaiian alphabet, the missionaries were able to teach Kanaka Maoli reading and writing; by the early 1830s the first missionary-controlled printing press was…
Descriptors: Orthographic Symbols, Printing, Alphabets, Diachronic Linguistics
Sauzeon, H.; Lestage, P.; Raboutet, C.; N'Kaoua, B.; Claverie, B. – Brain and Language, 2004
Developmental changes in children's verbal fluency were explored in this study. One hundred and forty children aged from 7 to 16 completed four verbal fluency tasks, each with a different the production criterion (letter, sound, semantic, and free). The age differences were analyzed both in terms of number of words produced, and clustering,…
Descriptors: Language Fluency, Age Differences, Developmental Stages, Semantics
Share, David L. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2004
Two experiments tested the common assumption that knowing the letter names helps children learn basic letter-sound (grapheme-phoneme) relation because most names contain the relevant sounds. In Experiment 1 (n=45), children in an experimental group learned English letter names for letter-like symbols. Some of these names contained the…
Descriptors: Alphabets, Phoneme Grapheme Correspondence, Experimental Groups, Control Groups