Publication Date
In 2025 | 16 |
Since 2024 | 59 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 286 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 816 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 1867 |
Descriptor
Source
Author
Publication Type
Education Level
Location
Turkey | 46 |
Canada | 44 |
United Kingdom | 44 |
Australia | 38 |
Netherlands | 31 |
Taiwan | 27 |
Germany | 26 |
Iran | 26 |
California | 24 |
United States | 22 |
New York | 20 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
No Child Left Behind Act 2001 | 3 |
Brown v Board of Education | 1 |
Higher Education Opportunity… | 1 |
Individuals with Disabilities… | 1 |
Individuals with Disabilities… | 1 |
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Does not meet standards | 3 |
Rivera, Ferdinand; Becker, Joanne Rossi – International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, 2003
In this report, we address the following questions: What aspects of information do preservice elementary teachers rely on when performing inductive reasoning? What contexts enable them to perceive the inherent invariant relationships from a finite sample and, thus, formulate viable generalizations? To what extent are they able to justify inductive…
Descriptors: Logical Thinking, Preservice Teachers, Cues, Models
Camburn, Eric; Correnti, Richard; Taylor, James – 2000
This paper presents the results of qualitative analyses that were designed to assess the validity of teachers' responses to items that measure topic coverage in mathematics and language arts. Using data from 12 "think-aloud" interviews and drawing on psychological theory about how respondents answer survey items, this research explored a number of…
Descriptors: Elementary Education, Elementary School Teachers, Interviews, Language Arts

van Gelderen, Amos – Written Communication, 1997
Studies revision skills of grades 5-6 students asked to improve expository text written by other students by giving explicit evaluations, diagnoses, and suggestions. Supplements quantitative data with qualitative analysis of revision activities. Specifies in the model important cognitive steps in revision: compare, diagnose, and operate. Concludes…
Descriptors: Critical Reading, Intermediate Grades, Models, Protocol Analysis

Sepassi, Forood – Indian Journal of Applied Linguistics, 2003
A model of information processing is used in conjunction with two verbal protocol elicitation formats--the think aloud model and the retrospective questionnaire--to shed light on the mental processes involved in a learner's performance on the cloze. Results suggest the cloze is more suitable for learners of higher language ability, because it is…
Descriptors: Cloze Procedure, Cognitive Processes, English (Second Language), Foreign Countries

Magliano, Joseph P.; Millis, Keith K. – Cognition and Instruction, 2003
Two studies examined the viability of assessing reading strategies using a think-aloud protocol combined with latent semantic analysis (LSA). Findings demonstrated that the responses of less-skilled readers semantically overlapped more with focal sentences than with causal antecedent sentences, whereas skilled readers' responses overlapped with…
Descriptors: College Students, Comparative Analysis, Performance Factors, Predictor Variables

Woodall, Billy R. – Journal of Second Language Writing, 2002
A protocol analysis of second language (L2) writing of adult speakers of Japanese, English, and Spanish observed how language switching (first language (L1) switching use in L2 writing) was affected by L2 proficiency, task difficulty, and language group. Less proficient L2 learners switched to their L1 more frequently than more advanced learners,…
Descriptors: Adults, Code Switching (Language), English, Japanese

Fontaine, Sheryl I. – Educational Research Quarterly, 1989
A three-part training procedure was developed to adapt thinking-aloud writing protocols (TAWPs) for use with children as verbal reports of their audience awareness. Four nine-year olds wrote letters while producing TAWPs. Children know that audience factors affect writers' decisions, but they do not use this awareness when making writing…
Descriptors: Audience Awareness, Case Studies, Children, Communication Skills

Kaplan, Craig A.; Simon, Herbert A. – Cognitive Psychology, 1990
Attaining the insight needed to solve the Mutilated Checkerboard problem, which requires discovery of an effective problem representation (EPR), is described. Performance on insight problems can be predicted from the availability of generators and constraints in the search for an EPR. Data for 23 undergraduates were analyzed. (TJH)
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Computer Simulation, Difficulty Level, Heuristics

Swanson, H. Lee; And Others – American Educational Research Journal, 1990
Qualitative differences between 24 expert teachers and 24 novice teachers in think-aloud protocols related to solving classroom discipline problems were investigated. Results suggest that expert teachers have procedural plans for solving discipline problems and are able to put more attention into defining the problem than are novice teachers. (SLD)
Descriptors: Beginning Teachers, Classroom Techniques, Cognitive Processes, Comparative Analysis

Bowen, Craig W. – Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 1990
Presented is an analysis of think-aloud protocols of graduate students solving several different tasks in organic chemistry. The systems used by these subjects were classified. It was reported that the methodological system was most often used. (CW)
Descriptors: College Science, Higher Education, Learning Strategies, Logical Thinking

Stiff, Lee V. – School Science and Mathematics, 1988
Problem solving by example is an intermediate step toward mastering problem-solving heuristics. Discussed are problem-solving processes by using two problem examples. Emphasized are heuristics cataloging in a problem-solving log, and approaches to solutions, not the solutions themselves. (YP)
Descriptors: Heuristics, Mathematical Concepts, Mathematics Achievement, Mathematics Education

Woods, Donald R. – Journal of College Science Teaching, 1988
Explains the differences between successful and unsuccessful problem solvers' exploration of a problem, translation of information into different forms, approach to devising and executing a plan, and rechecking work. (RT)
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, College Science, Concept Formation, Critical Thinking

Mack, Nancy K. – Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 1990
Examines the development of students' understandings about fractions during six weeks of instruction. Reports that all students possessed informal knowledge disconnected from their knowledge of fraction symbols and procedures and that knowledge of rote procedures often interfered with students' attempts to build on their informal knowledge.…
Descriptors: Arithmetic, Concept Formation, Concept Teaching, Elementary Education

Garrett, Roger M. – Research in Science and Technological Education, 1989
Reviewed are 15 studies on cognitive style and problem solving in science education. The effects of problem context, problem type, and three kinds of cognitive style on subjects' abilities to encounter and solve problems are investigated. Three protocols of the subjects' encountering activities are provided. (YP)
Descriptors: Cognitive Style, Field Dependence Independence, Foreign Countries, Physics

Smith, Mike U. – Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 1988
Examines successful/unsuccessful distinctions between novices and experts in problem solving in terms of genetic knowledge, use of production rules, strategy selection, use of critical cues, use of logic, understanding of probability, and the thinking process itself. Suggests five implications for genetics instruction and provides three problems…
Descriptors: Abstract Reasoning, Biology, College Science, Genetics