NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing 1,876 to 1,890 of 2,772 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bellon-Harn, Monica L.; Harn, William E.; Watson, Gina D. – Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 2007
A fundamental deficit in children with high-functioning autism (HFA) is social communicative competence. Atypical prosody in variable forms has been implicated in contributing to this deficit. The purpose of this case study was to describe the clinical management of an eight-year-old child with HFA for whom prosody became the primary target of…
Descriptors: Intervention, Communicative Competence (Languages), Autism, Interpersonal Communication
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bruckner, Cornelia; Yoder, Paul; Stone, Wendy; Saylor, Megan – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 2007
Purpose: To evaluate whether the validity of the Receptive Vocabulary scale of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory for Infants (MCDI-I; L. Fenson et al., 1991), a parent-report measure of early vocabulary, could be improved for children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) by removing items that are biased. Method: Logistic…
Descriptors: Construct Validity, Test Validity, Toddlers, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Stanley, Gillian C.; Konstantareas, M. Mary – Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2007
The relationship between symbolic play and other domains, such as degree of autistic symptomatology, nonverbal cognitive ability, receptive language, expressive language, and social development, was investigated. The assessment files of 101 children with Autism Spectrum Disorder were studied. Nonverbal cognitive ability and expressive language…
Descriptors: Cognitive Ability, Social Development, Receptive Language, Play
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Delinicolas, Erin K.; Young, Robyn L. – Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice, 2007
This study aimed to investigate the relationships between abilities to initiate and respond to joint attention and symptoms of autism that have, and have not, been theoretically linked to joint attention. Participants were 51 boys and five girls with autistic disorder, aged between 2 years and 6 years 5 months. Measures of joint attention…
Descriptors: Autism, Attention, Young Children, Social Behavior
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Strassman, Barbara K.; And Others – Journal of Communication Disorders, 1987
Two studies examined the ability of 22 deaf high school students to instantiate (select appropriate word meaning from context) particular exemplars of general nouns and to use those instantiations as retrieval cues. Among results were that subjects could instantiate when asked to do so but did not do so spontaneously. (Author/DB)
Descriptors: Comprehension, Deafness, High Schools, Receptive Language
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Carr, Edward G.; And Others – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1984
Studies two groups of autistic children--good versus poor verbal imitators--within the context of a receptive label acquisition task. Both groups acquired receptive signs. However, good imitators acquired receptive speech whereas poor imitators typically did not. (Author/AS)
Descriptors: Autism, Language Acquisition, Predictor Variables, Receptive Language
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Wolf, Maryanne – Annals of Dyslexia, 1984
Based on a neurolinguistic model of naming, naming and reading tests were administered to a longitudinal sample of 115 kindergarteners before, during, and after reading acquisition. Preliminary trends indicate that poor readers are significantly different from average readers on all naming tests except those emphasizing receptive vocabulary…
Descriptors: Dyslexia, Expressive Language, Kindergarten, Longitudinal Studies
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Vincent-Smith, Lisbeth; And Others – Child Development, 1974
The acquisition of language-receptive processes in preschool children was studied using a word association task. (ST)
Descriptors: Comprehension, Discrimination Learning, Language Acquisition, Preschool Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Nippold, Marilyn A.; Fey, Sandy H. – Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 1983
Although 12 preadolescents who had been diagnosed as language impaired during preschool performed as well as a control group on certain tests involving literal aspects of language and on a nonverbal intelligence test, they were deficient in understanding metaphoric sentences and in performing the cognitive task. (Author/CL)
Descriptors: Elementary Education, Figurative Language, Language Handicaps, Metaphors
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Raver, Sharon A. – Child Study Journal, 1980
Descriptors: Intervention, Language Processing, Mental Retardation, Receptive Language
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Karrass, J.; Braungart-Rieker, J.M. – Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology: An International Lifespan Journal, 2005
This study investigated whether shared parent-infant book reading at 4 and 8 months would be associated with subsequent language abilities at 12 and 16 months. Parents of 87 typically developing middle-class infants reported on the presence or absence of shared reading in the home; infant language abilities were measured through laboratory…
Descriptors: Infants, Receptive Language, Language Acquisition, Expressive Language
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Maestu, Fernando; Saldana, Cristobal; Amo, Carlos; Gonzalez-Hidalgo, Mercedes; Fernandez, Alberto; Fernandez, Santiago; Mata, Pedro; Papanicolaou, Andrew; Ortiz, Tomas – Brain and Language, 2004
Shift of the cortical mechanisms of language from the usually dominant left to the non-dominant right hemisphere has been demonstrated in the presence of large brain lesions. Here, we report a similar phenomenon in a patient with a cavernoma over the anterolateral superior temporal gyrus associated with epilepsy. Language mapping was performed by…
Descriptors: Brain Hemisphere Functions, Patients, Epilepsy, Receptive Language
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Guajardo, Nicole R.; Snyder, Gregory; Petersen, Rachel – Infant and Child Development, 2009
The present study included observational and self-report measures to examine associations among parental stress, parental behaviour, child behaviour, and children's theory of mind and emotion understanding. Eighty-three parents and their 3- to 5-year-old children participated. Parents completed measures of parental stress, parenting (laxness,…
Descriptors: Parenting Styles, Child Rearing, Cognitive Development, Child Behavior
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Selassie, G. Rejno-Habte; Viggedal, G.; Olsson, I.; Jennische, M. – Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 2008
We studied expressive and receptive language, oral motor ability, attention, memory, and intelligence in 20 6-year-old children with epilepsy (14 females, six males; mean age 6y 5mo, range 6y-6y 11mo) without learning disability, cerebral palsy (CP), and/or autism, and in 30 reference children without epilepsy (18 females, 12 males; mean age 6y…
Descriptors: Epilepsy, Autism, Learning Disabilities, Seizures
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Harwood, Michelle D.; Farrar, M. Jeffrey – British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2006
The relation between theory of mind and affective perspective taking was examined in a study with 42 three- to five-year-olds. Children completed tasks measuring affective perspective taking, theory of mind, and receptive language abilities. Significant positive correlations existed between overall affective perspective taking and theory of mind…
Descriptors: Theory of Mind, Perspective Taking, Young Children, Receptive Language
Pages: 1  |  ...  |  122  |  123  |  124  |  125  |  126  |  127  |  128  |  129  |  130  |  ...  |  185