Publication Date
In 2025 | 31 |
Since 2024 | 147 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 590 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1473 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 3074 |
Descriptor
Source
Author
Publication Type
Education Level
Audience
Practitioners | 1236 |
Teachers | 911 |
Researchers | 150 |
Students | 125 |
Parents | 32 |
Administrators | 13 |
Policymakers | 8 |
Community | 2 |
Media Staff | 2 |
Support Staff | 2 |
Location
Australia | 165 |
Turkey | 82 |
Canada | 62 |
China | 48 |
Germany | 46 |
Indonesia | 46 |
South Africa | 46 |
United States | 41 |
United Kingdom (England) | 39 |
Taiwan | 37 |
United Kingdom | 37 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Meets WWC Standards without Reservations | 18 |
Meets WWC Standards with or without Reservations | 22 |
Does not meet standards | 6 |
Purpura, David J.; Lonigan, Christopher J. – American Educational Research Journal, 2013
Validating the structure of informal numeracy skills is critical to understanding the developmental trajectories of mathematics skills at early ages; however, little research has been devoted to construct evaluation of the Numbering, Relations, and Arithmetic Operations domains. This study was designed to address this knowledge gap by examining…
Descriptors: Arithmetic, Numeracy, Mathematics Skills, Training Methods
Watson, Jane; English, Lyn – Australian Primary Mathematics Classroom, 2013
Jane Watson and Lyn English use a chance activity exploring expectation and variation with coin tossing to highlight the importance of understanding the part-whole relationship embodied in percentage and its power to measure and compare for different wholes, in this case different sample sizes. The purpose of this article is to raise awareness of…
Descriptors: Numbers, Probability, Mathematical Concepts, Mathematics Instruction
Landy, David; Silbert, Noah; Goldin, Aleah – Cognitive Science, 2013
Despite their importance in public discourse, numbers in the range of 1 million to 1 trillion are notoriously difficult to understand. We examine magnitude estimation by adult Americans when placing large numbers on a number line and when qualitatively evaluating descriptions of imaginary geopolitical scenarios. Prior theoretical conceptions…
Descriptors: Numbers, Computation, Adults, Models
Brooks, Neon; Audet, Jennifer; Barner, David – Developmental Psychology, 2013
Before children learn the specific meanings of numerals like six, do they know that they represent precise quantities? Previous studies have reported conflicting evidence and have found that children expect numerals to label precise quantities in some tasks but not in others (Condry & Spelke, 2008; Sarnecka & Gelman, 2004). In this…
Descriptors: Pragmatics, Inferences, Preschool Children, Numbers
Kiili, Kristian; Ojansuu, Kai; Lindstedt, Antero; Ninaus, Manuel – International Journal of Game-Based Learning, 2018
The main aim of this article was to investigate the educational potential of a game-based math game competition to engage students in training rational numbers. Finnish fourth (n = 59; M[subscript age] = 10.36) and sixth graders (n = 105; M[subscript age] = 12.34) participated in a math game competition relying on intra-classroom cooperation and…
Descriptors: Mathematics Instruction, Teaching Methods, Educational Games, Competition
Regalla, Michele; Peker, Hilal – Learning Languages, 2018
This paper addresses the issue of integrating students with special needs into foreign language programs in the early childhood years. A partnership between a university and a charter school in central Florida began as a result of parent and teacher interest in providing a foreign language program for its prekindergarten students. Since 2014,…
Descriptors: Second Language Learning, Second Language Instruction, Special Needs Students, French
Siegler, Robert S. – Developmental Science, 2016
The integrated theory of numerical development posits that a central theme of numerical development from infancy to adulthood is progressive broadening of the types and ranges of numbers whose magnitudes are accurately represented. The process includes four overlapping trends: (1) representing increasingly precisely the magnitudes of non-symbolic…
Descriptors: Numbers, Theories, Individual Development, Symbols (Mathematics)
Russo, James – Australian Primary Mathematics Classroom, 2016
The use of enabling and extending prompts allows tasks to be both accessible and challenging within a classroom. This article provides an example of how to use enabling and extending prompts effectively when employing a challenging task in Year 2.
Descriptors: Mathematics Instruction, Elementary School Mathematics, Teaching Methods, Mathematical Concepts
Niklas, Frank; Cohrssen, Caroline; Tayler, Collette – Early Education and Development, 2016
Research Findings: Young children develop numeracy competencies during interactions with more knowledgeable others. Such interactions typically occur in the home numeracy environment (HNE). In this study a nonintensive intervention procedure was developed to improve both the HNE and numerical competencies. All parents of 113 Australian children…
Descriptors: Numeracy, Preschool Children, Preschool Education, Family Environment
Agozie, Jayme Gonzales – ProQuest LLC, 2016
The purpose of this quantitative study was to examine the ability of standardized test scores to predict the performance of students in first-year mathematics courses, and the extent to which these tests displayed differential validity among various subgroups. Using discriminant analysis, it was established that the following percentages of…
Descriptors: College Entrance Examinations, Standardized Tests, Numbers, Hispanic American Students
Siegler, Robert S. – Grantee Submission, 2016
The integrated theory of numerical development posits that a central theme of numerical development from infancy to adulthood is progressive broadening of the types and ranges of numbers whose magnitudes are accurately represented. The process includes four overlapping trends: 1) representing increasingly precisely the magnitudes of non-symbolic…
Descriptors: Numbers, Theories, Individual Development, Symbols (Mathematics)
Khomais, Sama F. – Early Child Development and Care, 2014
This study aimed to present part of a mathematics programme for preschool age, to examine its efficiency in improving children's mathematical abilities in the area of number, and to investigate its suitability for Saudi practice. Pre- and post-tests, using TEMA-2 for one experimental and two control groups were conducted and quantitatively…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Intervention, Numbers, Number Concepts
Laski, Elida V.; Siegler, Robert S. – Developmental Psychology, 2014
We tested the hypothesis that encoding the numerical-spatial relations in a number board game is a key process in promoting learning from playing such games. Experiment 1 used a microgenetic design to examine the effects on learning of the type of counting procedure that children use. As predicted, having kindergartners count-on from their current…
Descriptors: Games, Numbers, Learning, Cognitive Processes
Kallai, Arava Y.; Tzelgov, Joseph – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2014
Common fractions have been found to be processed intentionally but not automatically, which led to the conclusion that they are not represented holistically in long-term memory. However, decimals are more similar to natural numbers in their form and thus might be better candidates to be holistically represented by educated adults. To test this…
Descriptors: Arithmetic, Cognitive Processes, College Students, Mathematics
O'Dell, Robin S. – Mathematics Teacher, 2014
The simple process of iteration can produce complex and beautiful figures. In this article, Robin O'Dell presents a set of tasks requiring students to use the geometric interpretation of complex number multiplication to construct linear iteration rules. When the outputs are plotted in the complex plane, the graphs trace pleasing designs…
Descriptors: Mathematics Instruction, Geometric Concepts, Multiplication, Graphs