Publication Date
In 2025 | 20 |
Since 2024 | 64 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 291 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 821 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 1872 |
Descriptor
Source
Author
Publication Type
Education Level
Location
Turkey | 46 |
Canada | 44 |
United Kingdom | 44 |
Australia | 38 |
Netherlands | 31 |
Taiwan | 27 |
Germany | 26 |
Iran | 26 |
California | 24 |
United States | 22 |
New York | 20 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
No Child Left Behind Act 2001 | 3 |
Brown v Board of Education | 1 |
Higher Education Opportunity… | 1 |
Individuals with Disabilities… | 1 |
Individuals with Disabilities… | 1 |
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Does not meet standards | 3 |

Magliano, Joseph P.; Millis, Keith K. – Cognition and Instruction, 2003
Two studies examined the viability of assessing reading strategies using a think-aloud protocol combined with latent semantic analysis (LSA). Findings demonstrated that the responses of less-skilled readers semantically overlapped more with focal sentences than with causal antecedent sentences, whereas skilled readers' responses overlapped with…
Descriptors: College Students, Comparative Analysis, Performance Factors, Predictor Variables

Woodall, Billy R. – Journal of Second Language Writing, 2002
A protocol analysis of second language (L2) writing of adult speakers of Japanese, English, and Spanish observed how language switching (first language (L1) switching use in L2 writing) was affected by L2 proficiency, task difficulty, and language group. Less proficient L2 learners switched to their L1 more frequently than more advanced learners,…
Descriptors: Adults, Code Switching (Language), English, Japanese

Fontaine, Sheryl I. – Educational Research Quarterly, 1989
A three-part training procedure was developed to adapt thinking-aloud writing protocols (TAWPs) for use with children as verbal reports of their audience awareness. Four nine-year olds wrote letters while producing TAWPs. Children know that audience factors affect writers' decisions, but they do not use this awareness when making writing…
Descriptors: Audience Awareness, Case Studies, Children, Communication Skills

Kaplan, Craig A.; Simon, Herbert A. – Cognitive Psychology, 1990
Attaining the insight needed to solve the Mutilated Checkerboard problem, which requires discovery of an effective problem representation (EPR), is described. Performance on insight problems can be predicted from the availability of generators and constraints in the search for an EPR. Data for 23 undergraduates were analyzed. (TJH)
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Computer Simulation, Difficulty Level, Heuristics

Swanson, H. Lee; And Others – American Educational Research Journal, 1990
Qualitative differences between 24 expert teachers and 24 novice teachers in think-aloud protocols related to solving classroom discipline problems were investigated. Results suggest that expert teachers have procedural plans for solving discipline problems and are able to put more attention into defining the problem than are novice teachers. (SLD)
Descriptors: Beginning Teachers, Classroom Techniques, Cognitive Processes, Comparative Analysis

Bowen, Craig W. – Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 1990
Presented is an analysis of think-aloud protocols of graduate students solving several different tasks in organic chemistry. The systems used by these subjects were classified. It was reported that the methodological system was most often used. (CW)
Descriptors: College Science, Higher Education, Learning Strategies, Logical Thinking

Stiff, Lee V. – School Science and Mathematics, 1988
Problem solving by example is an intermediate step toward mastering problem-solving heuristics. Discussed are problem-solving processes by using two problem examples. Emphasized are heuristics cataloging in a problem-solving log, and approaches to solutions, not the solutions themselves. (YP)
Descriptors: Heuristics, Mathematical Concepts, Mathematics Achievement, Mathematics Education

Woods, Donald R. – Journal of College Science Teaching, 1988
Explains the differences between successful and unsuccessful problem solvers' exploration of a problem, translation of information into different forms, approach to devising and executing a plan, and rechecking work. (RT)
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, College Science, Concept Formation, Critical Thinking

Mack, Nancy K. – Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 1990
Examines the development of students' understandings about fractions during six weeks of instruction. Reports that all students possessed informal knowledge disconnected from their knowledge of fraction symbols and procedures and that knowledge of rote procedures often interfered with students' attempts to build on their informal knowledge.…
Descriptors: Arithmetic, Concept Formation, Concept Teaching, Elementary Education

Garrett, Roger M. – Research in Science and Technological Education, 1989
Reviewed are 15 studies on cognitive style and problem solving in science education. The effects of problem context, problem type, and three kinds of cognitive style on subjects' abilities to encounter and solve problems are investigated. Three protocols of the subjects' encountering activities are provided. (YP)
Descriptors: Cognitive Style, Field Dependence Independence, Foreign Countries, Physics

Smith, Mike U. – Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 1988
Examines successful/unsuccessful distinctions between novices and experts in problem solving in terms of genetic knowledge, use of production rules, strategy selection, use of critical cues, use of logic, understanding of probability, and the thinking process itself. Suggests five implications for genetics instruction and provides three problems…
Descriptors: Abstract Reasoning, Biology, College Science, Genetics

Wilson, Marilyn; Thomas, Sharon – English Education, 1995
Responds to the common assumption among English teachers that prior knowledge of a subject enhances students' ability to comprehend materials that they read. Presents evidence from the think-aloud protocols of undergraduate students to argue that having prior experience of the subjects treated in texts does not ensure that students will make…
Descriptors: Cultural Context, Cultural Differences, English Instruction, Higher Education

Londner, Linda – Journal of Creative Behavior, 1991
Eight sixth grade gifted students verbalized their thoughts as they worked on figural task drawings. Analysis of resulting data indicated that the process variables of Story Weaving, Association Shifting, and Vantage were more frequently evidenced by subjects producing more original products, whereas Content Bound Linking was associated with…
Descriptors: Association (Psychology), Cognitive Processes, Creative Activities, Creativity

Muth, K. Denise – Contemporary Educational Psychology, 1992
To determine how middle school students cope with the demands of arithmetic word problems, 140 eighth graders (67 males and 73 females) had to solve problems modeled after those of the National Assessment of Educational Progress. Extraneous information and extra steps reduce the accuracy of students' word problem solutions. (SLD)
Descriptors: Arithmetic, Grade 8, Junior High School Students, Junior High Schools

Upton, Thomas A. – Journal of College Reading and Learning, 1998
Outlines a model for understanding the second-language reading process. Uses errors in comprehension to illustrate how the reading comprehension process works. Discusses pedagogical implications and suggestions for teaching students to develop and practice better comprehension strategies using think-alouds. (RS)
Descriptors: English (Second Language), Error Analysis (Language), Higher Education, Models