NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Laws, Policies, & Programs
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing 331 to 345 of 479 results Save | Export
Pellegrini, A. D.; Greene, Helen – 1980
The extent to which free play versus sequenced questioning conditions facilitates preschoolers' associative fluency was investigated in this study. Twenty-four children (12 boys and 12 girls, with a mean age of 50.7 months) were randomly assigned to one of three conditions: free play, sequenced questioning, and control. In the sequenced…
Descriptors: Association (Psychology), Comparative Analysis, Creativity, Object Manipulation
Bender, Bruce G.; Levin, Joel R. – 1975
The purpose of this study was to determine whether motor activity, previously assumed necessary to induce visual imagery in young children's associative learning, actually has to be executed in order for children to generate images. The results of an experiment with 96 kindergartners clearly suggest not: in conditions where subjects simply planned…
Descriptors: Associative Learning, Elementary Education, Imagery, Kindergarten Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Bornstein, R. A. – Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1986
The distribution of intermanual discrepancies on three motor tests (Grooved Pegboard, Finger Tapping and Dynamometer) were examined in normal and unilateral brain lesion samples. Considerable variability in the intermanual discrepancies was observed in all three samples. Therefore, the consistency of discrepancies across tasks was examined. The…
Descriptors: Brain Hemisphere Functions, Neurological Impairments, Neurological Organization, Neurology
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
McCune, Lorraine; Ruff, Holly A. – Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 1985
Research on object exploration by infants addresses milestones in the process by normally developing infants, including spontaneous manipulation strategies. Implications for developmentalists working with high-risk preterm infants are noted, including the need for direct assistance in manipulation and for selection of materials. (CL)
Descriptors: Child Development, Disabilities, High Risk Persons, Infants
Lambert, Martha Q. – Industrial Arts Vocational Education Supplement - Technical Education, 1970
Describes a two-year college-level program in air traffic control. (GR)
Descriptors: Aerospace Technology, Aviation Technology, Community Colleges, Electronic Equipment
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Rosenberg, Robin A.; Butler, Robert A. – Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 1982
The effectiveness of sound as a reinforcer of manipulative responses was studied with 50 infants four to 12 months old. Noisy toys were manipulated significantly more frequently than quiet ones by infants from six to 10 months old. (Author/SEW)
Descriptors: Auditory Evaluation, Auditory Stimuli, Hearing Impairments, Manipulative Materials
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Locher, Paul J. – Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1982
Sighted subjects assembled puzzles under separate conditions of visual-haptic perception and used vision and touch simultaneously to illustrate visual-type involvement and links in haptic encoding processes. A cognitive component in perceptions was found. When visual input was inadequate or independent of haptic perception, tactual information was…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Higher Education, Object Manipulation, Sensory Integration
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Gabbard, Carl – Journal of Physical Education, Recreation and Dance, 1988
Details are presented regarding the essential elements of an effective early childhood physical education curriculum. Components include movement awareness, fundamental locomotor skills, fundamental nonlocomotor skills, fundamental manipulative skills, and health-related fitness. (CB)
Descriptors: Early Childhood Education, Health Education, Motor Development, Movement Education
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Readdick, Christine A. – Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 1994
Observed drawing by toddlers and preschoolers using primary and standard markers, pencils, and crayons to determine the influence of implement diameter on children's drawing products, performances, and preferences. The relationship between drawing and early home manipulative experience was also investigated. Confirmed findings of previous studies…
Descriptors: Childhood Attitudes, Family Influence, Freehand Drawing, Media Selection
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Mandler, Jean M.; And Others – Cognitive Psychology, 1991
The conceptual categories that children have developed in their second year were studied in five experiments using object manipulation tasks. Subjects included 152 children from 18 to 31 months of age. These very young children had formed global conceptions of many domains of objects. (SLD)
Descriptors: Child Development, Classification, Cognitive Processes, Concept Formation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Rakison, David H.; Butterworth, George E. – Developmental Psychology, 1998
Two experiments used object-manipulation tasks to examine whether one- to two-year-olds form superordinate-like categories by attending to object parts. Findings indicated that 14- and 18-month-olds behaved systematically toward categories with different, but not matching, parts. Without part differences, none formed superordinate categories.…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Attention, Classification, Cognitive Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Orion, Judy – NAMTA Journal, 2001
Discusses the development of the human hand from birth to age three as it contributes to the formation of human personality. Considers how parallels in eye, hand, brain, and motor skill development portray the evolving complexity and adaptation of the human grasp and illustrate Montessori theories about the relationship between physical experience…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Early Experience, Infants, Montessori Method
Yung, Hsin I. – ProQuest LLC, 2008
The purpose of this study was to examine the effects of an animated agent that provides instructional scaffolding strategies via a story mnemonic or cuing question with feedback versus instructional scaffolding strategies alone on student achievement of different educational objectives in multimedia learning. Specifically, the study investigated…
Descriptors: Scaffolding (Teaching Technique), Academic Achievement, Educational Strategies, Prompting
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hadlington, Lee J.; Bridges, Andrew M.; Beaman, C. Philip – Brain and Cognition, 2006
Three experiments attempted to clarify the effect of altering the spatial presentation of irrelevant auditory information. Previous research using serial recall tasks demonstrated a left-ear disadvantage for the presentation of irrelevant sounds (Hadlington, Bridges, & Darby, 2004). Experiments 1 and 2 examined the effects of manipulating the…
Descriptors: Mental Computation, Human Body, Recall (Psychology), Serial Ordering
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Jones, Todd C.; Bartlett, James C.; Wade, Kimberley A. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2006
Conjunction errors occur when participants incorrectly identify as "old" novel test stimuli created by recombining parts of two study stimuli (parent items). Prior studies have reported that the conjunction error rate is higher when parent items are studied together than when they are studied apart (a parent proximity effect). In several…
Descriptors: Nonverbal Communication, Form Classes (Languages), Recognition (Psychology), Familiarity
Pages: 1  |  ...  |  19  |  20  |  21  |  22  |  23  |  24  |  25  |  26  |  27  |  ...  |  32