Publication Date
In 2025 | 80 |
Since 2024 | 266 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 923 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1966 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 3358 |
Descriptor
Source
Author
Tatsuoka, Kikumi K. | 21 |
Catran, Jack | 20 |
Al-Jarf, Reima | 12 |
Courville, Troy | 10 |
Dodd, Barbara | 10 |
Pine, Julian M. | 10 |
Treiman, Rebecca | 10 |
Borasi, Raffaella | 8 |
Bray, Melissa A. | 8 |
Dell, Gary S. | 8 |
Pan, Xingyu | 8 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Education Level
Audience
Practitioners | 209 |
Teachers | 148 |
Researchers | 139 |
Policymakers | 29 |
Administrators | 22 |
Students | 13 |
Counselors | 1 |
Media Staff | 1 |
Location
Turkey | 118 |
China | 77 |
Germany | 67 |
Australia | 66 |
Canada | 65 |
Saudi Arabia | 52 |
Indonesia | 48 |
Spain | 48 |
Japan | 42 |
United Kingdom | 42 |
United Kingdom (England) | 37 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Meets WWC Standards without Reservations | 1 |
Meets WWC Standards with or without Reservations | 1 |
Does not meet standards | 1 |
Botvinick, Matthew; Bylsma, Lauren M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2005
Previous research has shown that short-term memory for serial order can be influenced by background knowledge concerning regularities of sequential structure. Specifically, it has been shown that recall is superior for sequences that fit well with familiar sequencing constraints. The authors report a corresponding effect pertaining to serial…
Descriptors: Error Patterns, Short Term Memory, Prior Learning, Sequential Learning
Emons, Wilco H. M.; Sijtsma, Klaas; Meijer, Rob R. – Multivariate Behavioral Research, 2004
The person-response function (PRF) relates the probability of an individual's correct answer to the difficulty of items measuring the same latent trait. Local deviations of the observed PRF from the expected PRF indicate person misfit. We discuss two new approaches to investigate person fit. The first approach uses kernel smoothing to estimate…
Descriptors: Probability, Simulation, Item Response Theory, Test Items
Wang, Min; Koda, Keiko – Language Learning, 2005
This study examined word identification skills among Chinese and Korean college students learning to read English as a second language in a naming experiment and an auditory category judgment task. Both groups demonstrated faster and more accurate naming performance on high-frequency words than low-frequency words and on regular words than…
Descriptors: Second Language Learning, English (Second Language), Word Recognition, Asian Americans
Yoder, Paul J.; Tapp, Jon – Journal of Behavioral Education, 2004
Time-window sequential analyses test whether a target behavior occurs within a temporal window (e.g., within 2 seconds) after an antecedent behavior more than is expected by chance. This type of question is common when we need to know how one person or event may immediately affect another event or person in the natural environment. Theoretically,…
Descriptors: Monte Carlo Methods, Error Patterns, Association Measures, Effect Size
Bastiaanse, Roelien; Edwards, Susan – Brain and Language, 2004
The effect of two linguistic factors in Broca's and Wernicke's aphasia was examined using Dutch and English subjects. Three tasks were used to test (1) the comprehension and (2) the construction of sentences, where verbs (in Dutch) and verb arguments (in Dutch and English) are in canonical versus non-canonical position; (3) the production of…
Descriptors: Indo European Languages, Verbs, Word Order, Speech Impairments
Grondin, Simon – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2005
This study tested the hypothesis that memory is a major source of variance in temporal processing. Participants categorized intervals as short or long. The number of base durations and interval types mixed within blocks of trials varied from 1 session to another. Results revealed that mixing 2 base durations within blocks increased categorization…
Descriptors: Memory, Cognitive Processes, Hypothesis Testing, Intervals
Bjorness, Theresa E.; Tysor, Michael K.; Poe, Gina R.; Riley, Brett T. – Learning & Memory, 2005
We tested the hypothesis that rapid eye movement (REM) sleep is important for complex associative learning by restricting rats from entering REM sleep for 4 h either immediately after training on an eight-box spatial task (0-4 REMr) or 4 h following training (4-8 REMr). Both groups of REM-restricted rats eventually reached the same overall…
Descriptors: Cues, Eye Movements, Associative Learning, Animals
Brown-Schmidt, Sarah; Canseco-Gonzalez, Enriqueta – Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2004
In Mandarin Chinese, word meaning is partially determined by lexical tone (Wang, 1973). Previous studies suggest that lexical tone is processed as linguistic information and not as pure tonal information (Gandour, 1998; Van Lanker & Fromkin, 1973). The current study explored the online processing of lexical tones. Event-related potentials were…
Descriptors: Sentences, Cues, Syllables, Semantics
Gaffrey, Michael S.; Kleinhans, Natalia M.; Haist, Frank; Akshoomoff, Natacha; Campbell, Ashley; Courchesne, Eric; Muller, Ralph-Axel – Neuropsychologia, 2007
Language delay and impairment are salient features of autism. More specifically, there is evidence of atypical semantic organization in autism, but the functional brain correlates are not well understood. The current study used functional MRI to examine activation associated with semantic category decision. Ten high-functioning men with autism…
Descriptors: Visual Stimuli, Control Groups, Semantics, Autism
German, Diane J.; Newman, Rochelle S. – Reading Psychology, 2007
We examined how children with and without oral language (word-finding) difficulties (WFD) perform on oral reading (OR) versus silent reading recognition (SRR) tasks when reading the same words and how lexical factors influenced OR accuracy, error patterns, and nature of miscues. Primary-grade students were administered an experimental reading…
Descriptors: Silent Reading, Oral Reading, Oral Language, Familiarity
Ware, Elizabeth A.; Uttal, David H.; Wetter, Emily K.; DeLoache, Judy S. – Developmental Science, 2006
Prior research (DeLoache, Uttal & Rosengren, 2004) has documented that 18- to 30-month-olds occasionally make scale errors: they attempt to fit their bodies into or onto miniature objects (e.g. a chair) that are far too small for them. The current study explores whether scale errors are limited to actions that directly involve the child's…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Toys, Error Patterns, Young Children
Gershkoff-Stowe, Lisa; Connell, Brenda; Smith, Linda – Journal of Child Language, 2006
Overgeneralization occurs when a child uses the wrong word to name an object and is often observed in the early stages of word learning. We develop a method to elicit overgeneralizations in the laboratory by priming children to say the names of objects perceptually similar to known and unknown target objects. Experiment 1 examined 18 two-year-old…
Descriptors: Novelty (Stimulus Dimension), Language Processing, Language Acquisition, Young Children
Abu-Rabia, Salim; Taha, Haitham – Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2006
Most of the spelling error analysis has been conducted in Latin orthographies and rarely conducted in other orthographies like Arabic. Two hundred and eighty-eight students in grades 1-9 participated in the study. They were presented nine lists of words to test their spelling skills. Their spelling errors were analyzed by error categories. The…
Descriptors: Semitic Languages, Spelling, Phonology, Error Patterns
Smith, Michael Sharwood – 1996
Just as learning a first language is sometimes compared to existence within the relatively sheltered world of the Garden of Eden, the process of learning a second language is viewed as analogous to survival after expulsion from the Garden into a relatively harsh world, in which the learner must come to a conscious understanding of form and meaning…
Descriptors: Error Analysis (Language), Error Patterns, Interlanguage, Language Processing
Kanno, Yasuko – 1989
The use of connectives in 41 academic papers written for an English writing course by Japanese students ranging from university sophomores to graduate students was examined. Connectives were categorized as additive, adversative, causal, and temporal. Each connective type was subject to certain types of error. Additive connectives tended to be…
Descriptors: College Students, English for Academic Purposes, English (Second Language), Error Patterns