Publication Date
In 2025 | 259 |
Since 2024 | 931 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 2320 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 3942 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 7518 |
Descriptor
Source
Author
Publication Type
Education Level
Audience
Practitioners | 183 |
Teachers | 145 |
Researchers | 129 |
Administrators | 15 |
Parents | 12 |
Students | 9 |
Counselors | 4 |
Policymakers | 4 |
Support Staff | 2 |
Community | 1 |
Location
China | 201 |
Germany | 129 |
Australia | 119 |
Canada | 113 |
United Kingdom | 110 |
Netherlands | 100 |
Japan | 98 |
Spain | 97 |
Hong Kong | 63 |
France | 62 |
Turkey | 61 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
No Child Left Behind Act 2001 | 5 |
Education Consolidation… | 1 |
Head Start | 1 |
Individuals with Disabilities… | 1 |
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Meets WWC Standards without Reservations | 1 |
Meets WWC Standards with or without Reservations | 2 |
Does not meet standards | 3 |
Murty, Lalita; Otake, Takashi; Cutler, Anne – Language and Speech, 2007
Listeners rely on native-language rhythm in segmenting speech; in different languages, stress-, syllable- or mora-based rhythm is exploited. The rhythmic similarity hypothesis holds that where two languages have similar rhythm, listeners of each language should segment their own and the other language similarly. Such similarity in listening was…
Descriptors: Language Rhythm, Phonology, Dravidian Languages, Undergraduate Students
Freudenthal, Daniel; Pine, Julian M.; Gobet, Fernand – Journal of Child Language, 2007
P. Bloom's (1990) data on subject omission are often taken as strong support for the view that child language can be explained in terms of full competence coupled with processing limitations in production. This paper examines whether processing limitations in learning may provide a more parsimonious explanation of the data without the need to…
Descriptors: Form Classes (Languages), Child Language, Language Acquisition, Language Processing
Lukacs, Agnes; Pleh, Csaba; Racsmany, Mihaly – Journal of Child Language, 2007
We present data on the language of space in Hungarian individuals with Williams syndrome (WS; 19 in the first, 15 in the second study, between 8;0 and 21;11) and a verbal control (VC) group of typically developing (TD; 19 in the first, 15 in the second study, between 3;5 and 10;7) children from: (1) a study of elicited production and comprehension…
Descriptors: Mental Retardation, Spatial Ability, Interaction, Foreign Countries
Clahsen, Harald; Luck, Monika; Hahne, Anja – Journal of Child Language, 2007
This study examines the mental processes involved in children's on-line recognition of inflected word forms using event-related potentials (ERPs). Sixty children in three age groups (20 six- to seven-year-olds, 20 eight- to nine-year-olds, 20 eleven- to twelve-year-olds) and 23 adults (tested in a previous study) listened to sentences containing…
Descriptors: Sentences, Vocabulary Development, Brain, Language Processing
Holtgraves, Thomas – Language Learning, 2007
Recognizing the specific speech act ( Searle, 1969) that a speaker performs with an utterance is a fundamental feature of pragmatic competence. Past research has demonstrated that native speakers of English automatically recognize speech acts when they comprehend utterances (Holtgraves & Ashley, 2001). The present research examined whether this…
Descriptors: Speech Acts, Second Languages, Native Speakers, Pragmatics
Pienemann, Manfred; Hakansson, Gisela – Second Language Research, 2007
Ute Bohnacker's (2006) article on the acquisition of the verb second (V2) property in German by native speakers of Swedish (also a V2 language) is an attempted rebuttal of Hakansson et al.'s (2002) work on first language (L1) transfer and aspects of the underlying theory on which the work is based: Processability Theory (Pienemann, 1998). The…
Descriptors: Form Classes (Languages), Second Language Learning, Swedish, German
Love, Tracy E. – Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2007
Four experiments were performed which had the goal of determining how and when young children acquire the ability to understand long distance dependencies. These studies examined the operations underlying the auditory processing of non-canonically ordered constituents in object-relative sentences. Children 4-6 years of age and an adult population…
Descriptors: Sentences, Form Classes (Languages), Preschool Children, Language Processing
Ding, Yanren – System: An International Journal of Educational Technology and Applied Linguistics, 2007
Good language learner studies show that attending to form is associated with successful learning. This paper reports interviews with three university English majors who had won prizes in nationwide English speaking competitions and debate tournaments in China. The interviewees regarded text memorization and imitation as the most effective methods…
Descriptors: Imitation, Memorization, Foreign Countries, Linguistic Input
Demestre, Josep; Garcia-Albea, Jose E. – Cognitive Science, 2007
Event-related brain potentials were recorded while subjects listened to sentences containing a controlled infinitival complement. Subject and object control items were used, both with 2 potential antecedents in the upper clause. Half of the sentences had a gender agreement violation between the null subject of the infinitival complement and an…
Descriptors: Form Classes (Languages), Neurolinguistics, Language Processing, Error Analysis (Language)
Korpilahti, Pirjo; Jansson-Verkasalo, Eira; Mattila, Marja-Leena; Kuusikko, Sanna; Suominen, Kalervo; Rytky, Seppo; Pauls, David L.; Moilanen, Irma – Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2007
Many people with the diagnosis of Asperger syndrome (AS) show poorly developed skills in understanding emotional messages. The present study addressed discrimination of speech prosody in children with AS at neurophysiological level. Detection of affective prosody was investigated in one-word utterances as indexed by the N1 and the mismatch…
Descriptors: Suprasegmentals, Males, Asperger Syndrome, Language Processing
Clay, Felix; Bowers, Jeffrey S.; Davis, Colin J.; Hanley, Derek A. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2007
Semantic and orthographic learning of new words was investigated with the help of the picture-word interference (PWI) task. In this version of the Stroop task, picture naming is delayed by the simultaneous presentation of a semantically related as opposed to an unrelated distractor word (a specific PWI effect), as well as by an unrelated word…
Descriptors: Semantics, Vocabulary Development, Adults, Verbal Stimuli
Roland, Douglas; Dick, Frederic; Elman, Jeffrey L. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2007
Many recent models of language comprehension have stressed the role of distributional frequencies in determining the relative accessibility or ease of processing associated with a particular lexical item or sentence structure. However, there exist relatively few comprehensive analyses of structural frequencies, and little consideration has been…
Descriptors: Sentence Structure, Psycholinguistics, Grammar, Child Language
Fritsch, Thomas; McClendon, McKee J.; Smyth, Kathleen A.; Lerner, Alan J.; Friedland, Robert P.; Larsen, Janet D. – Gerontologist, 2007
Purpose: According to the "reserve perspective" on cognitive aging, individuals are born with or can develop resources that help them resist normal and disease-related cognitive changes that occur in aging. The reserve perspective is becoming more sophisticated, but gaps in knowledge persist. In the present research, we considered three…
Descriptors: Memory, Path Analysis, Intelligence Quotient, Cognitive Ability
Flaherty, Mary; Moran, Aidan – American Annals of the Deaf, 2007
Most studies on the Stroop effect (unintentional automatic word processing) have been restricted to English speakers using vocal responses. Little is known about this effect with deaf signers. The study compared Stroop task responses among four different samples: deaf participants from a Japanese-language environment and from an English-language…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Language Processing, Deafness, Sign Language
Corley, Martin; MacGregor, Lucy J.; Donaldson, David I. – Cognition, 2007
Everyday speech is littered with disfluency, often correlated with the production of less predictable words (e.g., Beattie & Butterworth [Beattie, G., & Butterworth, B. (1979). Contextual probability and word frequency as determinants of pauses in spontaneous speech. "Language and Speech, 22," 201-211.]). But what are the effects of disfluency on…
Descriptors: Listening Comprehension, Word Frequency, Speech Communication, Recognition (Psychology)