Publication Date
In 2025 | 245 |
Since 2024 | 917 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 2306 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 3927 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 7502 |
Descriptor
Source
Author
Publication Type
Education Level
Audience
Practitioners | 183 |
Teachers | 145 |
Researchers | 129 |
Administrators | 15 |
Parents | 12 |
Students | 9 |
Counselors | 4 |
Policymakers | 4 |
Support Staff | 2 |
Community | 1 |
Location
China | 198 |
Germany | 129 |
Australia | 118 |
Canada | 112 |
United Kingdom | 109 |
Netherlands | 99 |
Japan | 98 |
Spain | 96 |
Hong Kong | 63 |
France | 62 |
Iran | 60 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
No Child Left Behind Act 2001 | 5 |
Education Consolidation… | 1 |
Head Start | 1 |
Individuals with Disabilities… | 1 |
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Meets WWC Standards without Reservations | 1 |
Meets WWC Standards with or without Reservations | 2 |
Does not meet standards | 3 |
Wright, Heather Harris; Newhoff, Marilyn – Brain and Language, 2004
Processing abilities in aphasia, and the nature of processing breakdowns, were the focuses of this investigation. Individuals with either fluent or nonfluent aphasia, plus a control group, participated in a cross-modal lexical priming task designed to elicit priming effects when activation of inference interpretations occurred. Comprehension of…
Descriptors: Inferences, Control Groups, Aphasia, Language Processing
Ellis, Andrew W. – Brain and Language, 2004
It has long been known that the number of letters in a word has more of an effect on recognition speed and accuracy in the left visual field (LVF) than in the right visual field (RVF) provided that the word is presented in a standard, horizontal format. After considering the basis of the length by visual field interaction two further differences…
Descriptors: Word Recognition, Visual Perception, Eye Movements, Language Processing
Webber, Bonnie – Cognitive Science, 2004
This paper surveys work on applying the insights of lexicalized grammars to low-level discourse, to show the value of positing an autonomous grammar for low-level discourse in which words (or idiomatic phrases) are associated with discourse-level predicate-argument structures or modification structures that convey their syntactic-semantic meaning…
Descriptors: Grammar, Surveys, Lexicology, Discourse Analysis
Cholin, Joana; Schiller, Niels O.; Levelt, Willem J. M. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2004
Models of speech production assume that syllables play a functional role in the process of word-form encoding in speech production. In this study, we investigate this claim and specifically provide evidence about the level at which syllables come into play. We report two studies using an "odd-man-out" variant of the "implicit priming paradigm" to…
Descriptors: Speech, Speech Communication, Syllables, Reading Skills
Finkbeiner, Matthew; Forster, Kenneth; Nicol, Janet; Nakamura, Kumiko – Journal of Memory and Language, 2004
A well-known asymmetry exists in the bilingual masked priming literature in which lexical decision is used: namely, masked primes in the dominant language (L1) facilitate decision times on targets in the less dominant language (L2), but not vice versa. In semantic categorization, on the other hand, priming is symmetrical. In Experiments 1-3 we…
Descriptors: Language Processing, Language Dominance, Semantics, Models
Schiller, Niels O. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2004
This study investigates whether or not masked form priming effects in the naming task depend on the number of shared segments between prime and target. Dutch participants named bisyllabic words, which were preceded by visual masked primes. When primes shared the initial segment(s) with the target, naming latencies were shorter than in a control…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Cognitive Processes, Word Recognition, Indo European Languages
Nimmo, Lisa M.; Roodenrys, Steven – Journal of Memory and Language, 2004
The aim of the present research was to determine whether the effect that phonological similarity has on immediate serial recall is influenced by the consistency and position of phonemes within words. In comparison to phonologically dissimilar lists, when the stimulus lists rhyme there is a facilitative effect on the recall of item information and…
Descriptors: Recall (Psychology), Syllables, Phonemes, Phonology
Grossman, Murray; McMillan, Corey; Moore, Peachie; Ding, Lijun; Glosser, Guila; Work, Melissa; Gee, James – Brain, 2004
Confrontation naming is impaired in neurodegenerative conditions like Alzheimer's disease (AD), frontotemporal dementia (FTD) and corticobasal degeneration (CBD). Some behavioural observations suggest a common source of impaired naming across these patient groups, while others find partially unique patterns of naming difficulty. We hypothesized…
Descriptors: Brain, Dementia, Neurological Impairments, Alzheimers Disease
Nenonen, Sari; Shestakova, Anna; Huotilainen, Minna; Naatanen, Risto – Brain and Language, 2005
The mismatch negativity (MMN) component of the auditory event-related potential was used to determine the effect of native language, Russian, on the processing of speech-sound duration in a second language, Finnish, that uses duration as a cue for phonological distinction. The native-language effect was compared with Finnish vowels that either can…
Descriptors: Language Processing, Russian, Native Speakers, Finno Ugric Languages
Searl, Jeff; Ousley, Teri – Journal of Communication Disorders, 2004
Tracheoesophageal (TE) speakers often have difficulty producing the voiced-voiceless distinction. Phonation offset (POff) as a TE speaker transitions from a vowel to a stop consonant may be altered, possibly contributing to listener misperceptions. The purposes of this study were to: (1) compare the duration of POff in TE versus laryngeal…
Descriptors: Phonetics, Phonetic Analysis, Speech Communication, Language Processing
Haskell, Todd R.; MacDonald, Maryellen C. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2005
A number of studies have shown that structural factors play a much larger role than the linear order of words during the production of grammatical agreement. These findings have been used as evidence for a stage in the production process at which hierarchical relations between constituents have been established (a necessary precursor to…
Descriptors: Syntax, Verbs, Grammar, Language Processing
Christianson, Kiel; Johnson, Rebecca L.; Rayner, Keith – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2005
Three masked-prime naming experiments were conducted to examine the impact of morpheme boundaries on letter transposition confusability effects. In Experiment 1, the priming effects of primes containing letter transpositions within (sunhsine) and transpositions across (susnhine) the constituents of compound words were compared with correctly…
Descriptors: Morphemes, Alphabets, Spelling, Word Recognition
Eberhard, Kathleen M.; Cutting, J. Cooper; Bock, Kathryn – Psychological Review, 2005
Grammatical agreement flags the parts of sentences that belong together regardless of whether the parts appear together. In English, the major agreement controller is the sentence subject, the major agreement targets are verbs and pronouns, and the major agreement category is number. The authors expand an account of number agreement whose tenets…
Descriptors: Grammar, Morphemes, Structural Grammar, Verbs
Pexman, Penny M.; Trew, Jennifer L.; Holyk, Gregory G. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2005
The process of naming an exception word prime (e.g., PINT) delays subsequent naming of a nonrhyming regular-inconsistent word body neighbor target (e.g., TINT). Both an activation account (Taraban & McClelland, 1987) and a learning account (Burt & Humphreys, 1993) have been offered to explain this interference effect. We investigated how long…
Descriptors: Phoneme Grapheme Correspondence, Phonology, Cognitive Processes, Interference (Language)
Sentential Context and the Interpretation of Familiar Open-Compounds and Novel Modifier-Noun Phrases
Gagne, Christina L.; Spalding, Thomas L.; Gorrie, Melissa C. – Language and Speech, 2005
Two experiments investigated the influence of sentential context on the relative ease of deriving a particular meaning for novel and familiar compounds. Experiment 1 determined which of two possible meanings was preferred for a set of novel phrases. Experiment 2 used both novel (e.g., "brain sponge") and familiar compounds (e.g., "bug spray"). The…
Descriptors: Context Effect, Novelty (Stimulus Dimension), Familiarity, Nouns